Logo
    Search

    Sailors on a Dark Sea: Illusion of Explanatory Depth

    enJanuary 17, 2017

    Podcast Summary

    • The illusion of explanatory depthWe may believe we understand something, but have gaps in our knowledge. Recognize these gaps and strive to fill them through learning and exploration.

      Our perception of knowing something can sometimes be different from the reality. This was discussed in the context of the "illusion of explanatory depth," where we may believe we understand how something works or know a certain fact, but in reality, we may have gaps in our knowledge. This was illustrated using the example of trying to recall the name of an actor or character from a movie or TV show. However, this phenomenon can also apply to other areas of knowledge, such as understanding causal relationships or natural phenomena. It's important to recognize these gaps and strive to fill them through learning and exploration. Additionally, the episode touched on various topics such as wireless plans from Visible, the all-new Hyundai Santa Fe, co-owning luxury vacation homes with Picasso, and the importance of podcasts from AT&T.

    • Our understanding and feelings about understanding are separateThough we may have scientific explanations, our perception of the world can still hold magical or unexplained elements. The Socratic method can help bridge the gap between feeling and knowing.

      Our perception of the world around us, even when we have a scientific explanation for its workings, can still hold magical or unexplained elements. This is because our understanding and our feelings about understanding are separate. We often have a loose concept of magic or the supernatural, which can be seen as causal indeterminacy. For instance, a child might not understand the science behind a long-lasting helium balloon, but instead label it as "magic helium." Similarly, there are complex systems in our lives, like toilets or mirrors, that we use daily but may not fully comprehend. The Socratic teaching method, which encourages students to discover their own understanding, can help bridge the gap between feeling like we understand something and actually understanding it. However, there are also instances where we feel we understand something but do not, and research is ongoing to address the vast amount of knowledge in this category. Ultimately, the world is filled with mysteries, both big and small, and our ongoing quest for understanding is what keeps us curious.

    • Assumptions about mirrors and sightPeople often assume mirror reflections accurately represent our size and angles, but this is not always the case. Perception of sight can be influenced by fictional scenarios, leading to confusion between fact and fiction.

      Our understanding of how mirrors and sight work is not as solid as we might think. Despite our everyday encounters with these phenomena, we often struggle to predict how they behave or accurately perceive their reflections. A study from the University of Liverpool revealed that people tend to assume their reflection's size and angles are the same as their own, but this is not the case. Additionally, our perception of sight is influenced by fictional scenarios, leading to confusion between what we know intellectually and what we feel. Mirrors and sight are examples of the illusion of explanatory depth, where we believe we have a better grasp on a concept than we actually do. These findings highlight the importance of questioning our assumptions and being aware of the ways our perception can be influenced by cultural narratives and fictional scenarios.

    • Folk theories can be misleading, but we all use theories to understand the worldBeing aware of cognitive limitations and the mistakes our brains can make is crucial for accurate understanding of the world, even if some theories feel intuitive or explanatory, they may not be correct. Use tools like LifeLock to protect against identity theft and explore future tech trends on Technically Speaking podcast.

      Our intuitive understanding of the world, or folk theories, can often be misleading and provide an illusion of explanatory depth. For instance, the common belief that blue blood exists in our veins is an example of such a folk theory. However, it's essential to acknowledge that everyone, including scientists, use theories to explain the world around us. The study "Misunderstood Limits of Folk Science: An Illusion of Explanatory Depth" highlights the importance of being aware of our cognitive limitations and the mistakes our brains can make. The study also emphasizes that just because a theory feels intuitive or explanatory, it doesn't necessarily mean it's correct. The episode also touched upon identity theft protection and the importance of using tools like LifeLock to safeguard against potential threats. Additionally, it introduced Technically Speaking, an Intel podcast that explores the future of technology and AI.

    • People often overestimate their understanding of how things workPeople have shallow understandings of many phenomena and often overestimate their knowledge, leading to a false sense of explanatory depth.

      Our understanding of how things work may not be as deep or accurate as we believe it to be. This was highlighted in a study that discussed the difference between scientific theories, which are constantly being refined and improved, and folk theories, which are the explanatory systems produced in the minds of laypeople. The authors of the study noted that people have shallow understandings of many phenomena and fail to recognize the limits of their knowledge. This can lead to a false sense of explanatory depth. For example, we might think we understand how a cell phone works, but upon closer examination, we might realize that our understanding is incomplete or even incorrect. This can be problematic, as if we don't truly understand a concept, it's unlikely that we can explain it effectively to others. The study also noted that people are more overconfident about their understanding of how things work than they should be. So, the next time you find yourself explaining a concept and realizing you don't know it as well as you thought, don't be too hard on yourself. It's a reminder that our understanding is always a work in progress.

    • People overestimate their knowledge in some areas, underestimate it in othersIndividuals should assess their understanding of various types of knowledge and recognize areas of overconfidence or underconfidence for continuous improvement

      Our understanding of our own knowledge can be overestimated, and this overestimation varies depending on the type of knowledge. The authors of a large-scale study explored this concept, conducting 12 studies to measure people's confidence in their understanding versus their actual understanding across various domains of knowledge. They found that individuals tend to overestimate their knowledge in some areas while underestimating it in others. The authors argue against the idea of general knowledge and instead advocate for categorizing knowledge into different types. For instance, someone might know the capital of England with great confidence but struggle to explain how a lightsaber works. This illusion of explanatory depth was documented in Study 1, where participants were asked to rate their understanding of how various devices work. They were then asked to write detailed explanations and rate their initial understanding again. The authors found that participants often overestimated their initial understanding. These findings suggest that individuals may benefit from periodically assessing their understanding of different types of knowledge and recognizing the areas where they are overconfident or underconfident. This awareness can help individuals improve their understanding and make more informed decisions.

    • Underestimating our understanding until challengedConfronting what we don't know is crucial for accurate self-assessment, but feeling humbled doesn't always lead to acceptance of expert explanations

      Our understanding of complex concepts is often overestimated until we are challenged to explain them or receive expert explanations. The results of an experiment involving 16 Yale graduate students showed that participants initially overestimated their understanding of various devices, but their self-evaluations dropped significantly after they had to give explanations and answer diagnostic questions. However, the ratings did not continue to drop and instead rose after reading expert explanations. This pattern suggests that confronting what we don't know is necessary for accurate self-assessment. Despite feeling humbled by their lack of knowledge, some participants still clung to the illusion of explanatory depth and believed they would have performed better with different devices. The experiment was repeated with a larger sample of 33 Yale undergrads to confirm the results, but more research is needed to solidify the findings.

    • Study challenges graduate students' intellectual arroganceBoth undergraduates and graduates overestimate their understanding, but graduates show a slightly stronger illusion of explanatory depth. This challenges the theory of graduate students having more intellectual arrogance.

      Undergraduates and graduates may not be as overconfident about their understanding as previously thought. A study found that both undergraduates and graduates showed a similar pattern of overestimating their knowledge, followed by a decrease in confidence after being asked to explain, and then an increase in confidence after receiving expert explanations. However, the undergraduates showed a slightly stronger illusion of explanatory depth effect. This suggests that the theory of graduate students having more intellectual arrogance may be disproved. The study also replicated the results with students from a less selective university, who had slightly higher initial ratings of knowledge but showed a similar pattern of results. It's important to remember that overestimating one's understanding is a common experience, and it's not a sign of being a bad person. Instead, we should strive to be humbled by what we don't know and continually seek new knowledge.

    • The illusion of explanatory depth effect is robust across devices and hard to eliminateDespite warnings, people overestimate their understanding of how things work and believe they can explain it, which may not be solely driven by a conscious desire to appear knowledgeable.

      The illusion of explanatory depth effect, which refers to people overestimating their understanding of how things work before being asked to explain it, is robust across various types of devices and seems to be a genuine phenomenon. However, a warning that participants will be required to give an explanation for how an item works can reduce the effect's magnitude but doesn't eliminate it entirely. Participants' initial confidence ratings remain unchanged upon receiving the warning, but their later self-ratings become more optimistic. This suggests that the effect may not be solely driven by a conscious desire to appear knowledgeable. Instead, it could be influenced by unconscious factors, such as the priming effect or the desire for consistency in self-perception. The findings underscore the importance of considering the role of explanatory depth in assessing people's understanding and the potential implications for educational and instructional design.

    • Illusion of explanatory depth and overconfidence in understanding complex phenomenaThe illusion of knowing more than we do about complex topics may be a result of overconfidence or a distinct phenomenon, requiring further study.

      The illusion of explanatory depth, or overconfidence in one's understanding of complex phenomena, may not be unique to explanations, but could also apply to various types of knowledge, including facts, narratives, and procedures. The study discussed in the podcast suggests that participants may have tried to be more consistent with their ratings after receiving explicit instructions, leading to a replication of the initial effect. However, it also raises questions about whether this illusion of knowledge is a general overconfidence effect or a distinct phenomenon. The researchers plan to explore this further in future studies. Additionally, the podcast touches on the role of technology, particularly artificial intelligence, in shaping our future and the importance of understanding its potential and limitations.

    • Overconfidence in Knowledge and ProceduresStudents may be overconfident in their knowledge of facts and procedures due to lack of understanding of concepts. Focusing on understanding concepts can lead to more accurate self-assessment.

      Our brains have a tendency to overestimate our knowledge and confidence, especially when it comes to facts and procedures. In the discussed study, college undergraduates were tested on their knowledge of country capitals and device explanations. The results showed that participants were more overconfident when providing explanations, indicating a deeper understanding, compared to listing facts or procedures. However, the difference in confidence levels between the initial rating and the final rating was smaller for facts and procedures compared to explanations. This suggests that our education system, which often emphasizes memorization of facts and procedures without context or substance, may contribute to this overconfidence. It would be more beneficial for students to focus on understanding the concepts behind the facts and procedures rather than just memorizing them. This could lead to a more accurate assessment of one's knowledge and confidence.

    • Understanding the difference between recalling procedures and factsWriting down the steps to perform a task increases confidence, as our brains recall procedures differently than facts, and first-person memories of procedures can be vivid even if we can't describe them accurately.

      The way our brains process and recall information for procedures is different from how we recall facts or narratives. According to a study, when people were asked to write down the steps to perform a task, they showed increased confidence in their knowledge, even though the instructions might not have been complete. This is likely because when we remember how to do something, it's often a first-person memory, and we can picture ourselves doing it, even if we don't have the language to describe the steps accurately. For example, we might be able to build something with Legos or ride a bicycle, but have difficulty explaining the steps involved in those tasks to someone else. This phenomenon was not fully reflected in the study results, but it's a common experience. The study also compared the recall of facts and procedures, and found that the ratings for procedures showed a slight increase in confidence after writing down the steps, while the ratings for facts showed the usual drop-off between the initial guess and the adjusted rating. Another interesting finding was that people seemed more aware of their lack of knowledge about procedures compared to facts, and did not show the same level of surprise after the test. Overall, the study highlights the importance of understanding the different ways our brains process and recall information, and how this can impact learning and education.

    • People overestimate their understanding of various types of knowledgePeople are often overconfident in their knowledge of how devices work and natural phenomena, but less so with procedures and narratives.

      People's confidence in their understanding of various types of knowledge can be overestimated, and this overconfidence becomes more apparent when asked to explain or summarize that knowledge. In a series of studies, participants were asked to rate their understanding of different types of knowledge, such as machine procedures, geographical facts, movie plots, and natural phenomena. The results showed that people were most overconfident in their understanding of how devices work and how natural phenomena occur, while they were fairly accurate in judging their own knowledge of procedures and narratives. The study with movie plots was particularly interesting, as the results showed that people's understanding of movie plots was closer to the pattern for procedures than for devices. This could be due to the fact that recalling a narrative doesn't require physical action like explaining how a device works. However, natural phenomena were found to be similar to devices and machines in terms of having causal relationships and components that work together to produce an outcome. Overall, these studies highlight the importance of recognizing the limitations of our own knowledge and the value of seeking out additional information and clarification when needed.

    • Overestimating Our Understanding of Complex SystemsAssuming a complete grasp of complex systems based on high-level knowledge can lead to misunderstandings and hinder problem-solving abilities.

      Our understanding of complex systems, like household appliances or machinery, can be overestimated based on their appearance and our high-level knowledge of their functions. We often assume that we have a comprehensive understanding of how they work, but in reality, there are intricacies and complexities at lower levels that we may not be aware of. This phenomenon is known as the confusion of higher and lower levels of analysis. For instance, we might know how a bicycle works at a high level, but not have a clear understanding of the various parts and their functions. Similarly, we might understand the basic concept of car brakes, but not be able to explain the hydraulic systems and other intricacies involved. This overestimation of our knowledge can lead to a false sense of confidence and hinder our ability to troubleshoot or repair these systems when necessary.

    • The illusion of understanding complex systemsPeople may overestimate their knowledge of complex systems due to the transparency effect, where visible parts give a false sense of understanding

      The act of explaining how things work can be more complex and uncertain than recalling facts or procedures. Unlike facts, which have definitive answers, explanations can be open-ended and amorphous, leading to an illusion of depth of understanding. A study explored the possibility that some knowledge types are more socially desirable than others, potentially leading to overestimation of knowledge in those areas. However, the results showed no correlation between overconfidence and the social desirability of the knowledge domain. Instead, the researchers found that visible parts in devices are the most likely factor contributing to overconfidence in understanding how they work. This "transparency effect" can lead individuals to believe they understand a system better than they actually do.

    • The illusion of explanatory depth from mental movies and labelsOur mental models of the world may be less vivid and detailed than we assume, leading to an illusion of explanatory depth. Acknowledging this limitation can provide valuable insights and improve our understanding of complex concepts.

      Our understanding of how things work is not as complete as we might think based on our knowledge of their parts and labels. The confusion between different levels of analysis and label mechanism can lead to an illusion of explanatory depth. This illusion can be reinforced by our mental movies or mental representations, which can be less vivid than we assume and may lack the reality constraints of the actual object. This discovery, while surprising, can be seen as a reminder of the limitations of our mental models and the importance of recognizing their shallowness. Instead of being pessimistic about this, we can also view it optimistically, appreciating the ways in which we are able to survive and navigate the world despite the sparse nature of our mental imagery. Additionally, understanding this illusion can lead to valuable insights in various domains of life, such as education, politics, and even the supernatural.

    • Exploring the unknown and expanding our knowledgeContinually seeking new information and understanding the depths of our ignorance is essential for personal growth and knowledge expansion

      No matter how much we think we know, there's always more to learn. Even if we've seen a movie like "Big Trouble in Little China" before, there's a chance we'll discover new details upon rewatching it. Similarly, engaging in educational content, like Stuff to Blow Your Mind, can broaden our understanding of familiar topics. We're all continually exploring new topics and confronting what we don't know. The importance of this quest for knowledge is central to what we do. If you're interested in joining us on this journey, check out Stuff to Blow Your Mind's website for podcast episodes, videos, blog posts, and social media accounts. Remember, we're all just striving to understand the depths of our ignorance. So, don't hesitate to explore the unknown and expand your knowledge. For more information, visit Stuff to Blow Your Mind's website, or get in touch with us directly at [blowthemind@howstuffworks.com](mailto:blowthemind@howstuffworks.com).

    Recent Episodes from Stuff To Blow Your Mind

    Smart Talks with IBM: AI & the Productivity Paradox

    Smart Talks with IBM: AI & the Productivity Paradox

    In a rapidly evolving world, we need to balance the fear surrounding AI and its role in the workplace with its potential to drive productivity growth. In this special live episode of Smart Talks with IBM, Malcolm Gladwell is joined onstage by Rob Thomas, senior vice president of software and chief commercial officer at IBM, during NY Tech Week. They discuss “the productivity paradox,” the importance of open-source AI, and a future where AI will touch every industry.

    This is a paid advertisement from IBM. The conversations on this podcast don't necessarily represent IBM's positions, strategies or opinions.

    Visit us at ibm.com/smarttalks

    See omnystudio.com/listener for privacy information.

    Weirdhouse Cinema: The Dungeonmaster

    Weirdhouse Cinema: The Dungeonmaster

    In this episode of Weirdhouse Cinema, Rob and Joe return to the glorious world of 80s Charles Band productions with 1984’s “The Dungeonmaster,” a supernatural dreamscape with eight directors starring Jeffrey Byron, Richard Moll and Leslie Wing. It’s time to reject the devil’s reality and substitute your own! 

    See omnystudio.com/listener for privacy information.

    New Season of Smart Talks with IBM Coming Soon

    New Season of Smart Talks with IBM Coming Soon

    Smart Talks with IBM returns with a new season on Tuesday, June 25th. This season, we’re diving back into the world of artificial intelligence, but with a focus on the powerful concept of “open,” and how it applies to AI. Host Malcolm Gladwell will invite curious colleagues from Pushkin Industries to explore how openness is already reshaping industries, ways of doing business, and our very notion of what's possible.

    This is a paid advertisement from IBM.

    Visit us at ibm.com/smarttalks

    See omnystudio.com/listener for privacy information.

    Related Episodes

    136. Maps of Meaning 08: Neuropsychology of Symbolic Representation

    136. Maps of Meaning 08: Neuropsychology of Symbolic Representation

    In this lecture, Dr. Peterson discusses the relationship between the basic categories of imagistic/symbolic representation and brain function, noting that the very hemispheres of the brain are adapted, right/left to the environmental or experiential permanence of chaos/order or unexplored/explored territory, with consciousness serving the Logos role of communicative explorer (a function related in one of its deepest manifestations to the function of the hypothalamically grounded dopaminergic systems).

    -

    Thanks to our sponsors:

    https://helixsleep.com/jordan

    -

    Listen to The Mikhaila Peterson Podcast! https://podcasts.apple.com/us/podcast/the-mikhaila-peterson-podcast/id1514043751

    Your Behavior

    Your Behavior
    My next guest is Dr. Richard Pfau. He came to understand human behavior the long way.
     
    He started in the "hard sciences" at the Baltimore Polytechnic Institute and studied engineering at Johns Hopkins. He spent several years with the U.S. Naval Engineering Experiment Station helping to silence submarines and other ships. He got his bachelors in psychology from the University of Baltimore, graduating summa cum laude by the way, and went on to the Peace Corps in Nepal. That experience led to him to a PhD in science education from the University of Pittsburgh. Then more work in Nepal and 11 years in Africa plus teaching at the University of Connecticut and private consulting as a human resource development specialist. He has been around the block.
     
    Rich's many years of work and research have been aimed directly at understanding human behavior and have culminated in his new book "Your Behavior - Understanding and Changing the Things You Do." 
     
    In this episode we talk about how you can:
    1. analyze your behavior and change it if you want to
    2. understand why others behave the way they do
    3. see how your perceptions direct your behavior
    4. get more control of your life

    More about Rich and his work at:
    https://richardpfau.com/
    https://www.linkedin.com/in/richardpfau/

    SAM 106 | Sensory conflict can impact situational awareness

    SAM 106 | Sensory conflict can impact situational awareness

    The process by which situational awareness is formed begins withusing your five senses (seeing, hearing, feeling, tasting andsmelling) to gather information from your environment. That mayarguably be the easiest part of the situational awarenessdevelopmental process so long as you are “paying attention” to yourenvironment. Once the senses gather information, it converts itinto an electrical code that crackles its way down neural pathwaysto various destinations.

    At some point, the inputs from the senses must be combined,compared and contrasted to help achieve a coherent understanding ofwhat is happening. This “understanding” is the second step alongthe way to developing situational awareness. But it’s not alwayseasy to understand the meaning of information, especially whenthere is conflict among the inputs.

     

    Our sponsor:

    Midwest Fire
    MidwestFire.com

     

    Intro music

    Safety Dance (1982)

    Men Without Hats

    GMC - Virgin Records

     

    Situational Awareness Matters! website

    www.SAMatters.com

     

    Firefighter Near Miss Reporting System

    http://www.firefighternearmiss.com/

     

    Contact Rich Gasaway

    www.RichGasaway.com

    Support@RichGasaway.com

    612-548-4424

    SAM 085 | What is situational awareness?

    SAM 085 | What is situational awareness?

    In this episode we provide a working definition of situational awareness and take the mystery out of how it is developed and maintained by individuals working in high risk, high consequence environments.

    Specifically, we will discuss:

    • Perception
    • Understanding
    • Prediction
    • Intuition
    • Pattern Matching
    • Tacit Knowledge

    And we will address some of the reasons why situational awareness can be so challenging to develop and maintain.

    Our sponsor:

    Midwest Fire
    MidwestFire.com

     

    Intro music

    Safety Dance (1982)

    Men Without Hats

    GMC - Virgin Records

     

    Situational Awareness Matters! website

    www.SAMatters.com

     

    Firefighter Near Miss Reporting System

    http://www.firefighternearmiss.com/

     

    Contact Rich Gasaway

    www.RichGasaway.com

    Support@RichGasaway.com

    612-548-4424