Logo
    Search

    Podcast Summary

    • Memory follows a forgetting curveMemory retention decreases rapidly over time, but some information is still retained long-term. Active efforts to remember can help improve retention.

      Our memory is not a perfect diary that we can open and review at will, but rather, it follows a specific forgetting curve. This was discovered over a century ago by German philosopher Herman Ebbinghaus through his experiment on memorizing and forgetting thousands of random three-letter syllables. He found that memory retention decreases rapidly over time, with a significant drop-off within the first week and month. However, some information is still retained even months later. This research not only explains the natural forgetting process but also highlights the importance of actively trying to remember what we have forgotten, as it is often easier to relearn previously forgotten information than to learn it for the first time. This discovery marked the beginning of the scientific study of memory and its amenable nature to measurement and precision.

    • The Complexity of Memory: From Ebbinghaus to BartlettMemory is not just about recalling information verbatim, but a complex process influenced by our prior experiences and cultural understanding, involving both retention and reconstruction.

      Memory is not a simple process of recalling information verbatim. Instead, it is a complex construct that involves the interaction between new information and our existing knowledge. Herman Ebbinghaus was one of the first researchers to operationalize the study of memory in a laboratory setting, focusing on how we learn and forget information. He emphasized the importance of defining the construct of memory and developing appropriate tasks to study it. Later, Frederick Bartlett conducted an experiment on the War of the Ghosts story, revealing that forgetting is not random but rather influenced by our prior experiences and cultural understanding. The subjects in the experiment altered the story to fit their own experiences, showing that memory is a reconstructive process. Thus, memory is shaped by what we already know, and new information is incorporated into our existing knowledge base. This means that our memories are not just about the things we remember but also about the context in which we remember them and the connections we make between new and old information. Ultimately, memory is a dynamic and interactive process that involves both retention and reconstruction.

    • Our memories are like puzzles that get reconstructed from fragmentsResearch shows our memories can be easily influenced and distorted, leading to inaccuracies and false memories

      Our memories are like puzzles that we piece together from fragments, much like a paleontologist reconstructs a dinosaur skeleton from fossils. However, not all pieces are present, and we fill in the gaps with our best guesses and prior experiences. This process is both wonderful and terrifying as it allows us to remember past events but also leaves room for distortions and inaccuracies. Over the past 25 years, researchers have conducted numerous experiments demonstrating how easily our memories can be influenced and even distorted through suggestion techniques, such as being told about false events or being shown manipulated photos. These studies have shown that people can come to believe they experienced things that never occurred, like taking a hot air balloon ride or spilling punch on someone at a wedding. These findings highlight the suggestive nature of pictures and the ease with which our memories can be altered. Therefore, it's important to be aware of this mental paleontology process and the potential for inaccuracies in our memories.

    • The fallibility of memoriesMemories can change over time and are influenced by the way questions are phrased, casting doubt on their accuracy in various contexts

      Our memories are not always reliable. The discussion highlighted the fallibility of memories through personal experiences and scientific research. Ayana shared an anecdote about how she and her family have different recollections of an event from her childhood, emphasizing how memories can change over time. This phenomenon was further explored through research conducted by Elizabeth Loftus and John Palmer in the 1970s. Their experiments showed that the way questions are phrased can significantly influence people's recollections of witnessed events. For instance, changing a single word in a question about a car accident led participants to recall different speeds for the cars involved. Additionally, the presence or absence of certain words in questions influenced participants' memories of seeing broken glass at the scene of the accident, even when no broken glass was present in the actual event. These findings have serious implications, as they call into question the accuracy of memories in various contexts, including criminal justice proceedings. Therefore, it is crucial to be aware of the fallibility of our memories and the potential biases that can influence them.

    • Impact of suggestive questions on memoriesStudy reveals suggestive questions can lead to false memories, impacting criminal justice and the reliability of witness testimonies

      The way questions are asked and information is presented can significantly impact our memories, even influencing us to recall events that didn't actually occur. A study discussed on Hidden Brain revealed that participants' recollections were distorted when asked suggestive questions, leading to false memories of broken glass being present in a scene where none was. This phenomenon has important implications for criminal justice and the reliability of witness testimonies, as well as for jurors who may be influenced by media reports that can create more vivid and dramatic scenes. Ayana, the researcher, explained that these memories, both real and reconstructed, can become indistinguishable over time. The study highlights the need for understanding the fallibility of our memories and the potential impact of suggestive questioning and post-event information on our recollections.

    • The more we imagine something, the more likely we believe it happenedImagination can influence our memories, making us believe bizarre actions occurred even if they didn't. Our brains use past experiences to construct memories, but they may not always be accurate.

      Our memories are not always accurate depictions of reality. In a study, people were asked to imagine performing unusual actions repeatedly, and later, they were more likely to believe they had actually performed those actions, even if they were bizarre. This shows that the more times we imagine something, the more likely we are to believe it happened. However, it was easier for people to remember common actions than unusual ones. This suggests that our brains use elements of our past experiences to construct memories, even if they are not entirely accurate. This can lead to different people having vastly different recollections of the same event. It's important to keep in mind that memories are often suggestive and can be influenced by the stories we tell ourselves and others. In the context of news stories involving people's recollections of events, it's crucial to remember that memories can be fallible and may not align with the objective truth.

    • Memories can be subjective and distortedMemories aren't always accurate representations of past events, and individuals may recall them differently despite their best efforts.

      Memories, even vivid ones, are not always accurate representations of past events. The case of Christine Blasey Ford and Brett Kavanaugh's conflicting memories of an alleged assault serves as a reminder that memories can degrade and be distorted differently for individuals. It's essential to understand that both individuals could have been recalling the event as accurately as they could, but their memories may not align due to the subjective nature of memory. Believing that the more vividly someone recalls an event, the more it must have happened, is a misconception. The truth is, there's no definitive way to determine what exactly happened in the past, and it's essential to recognize that both parties may have their versions of the truth. As the Rashomon movie illustrates, different perspectives on an event do not necessarily mean that one person is lying.

    • The Impact of Perspective and Psychological Factors on MemoryPerspective and psychological factors like anxiety and threat can influence memory, while techniques like the memory palace can enhance it.

      Our perspective and biases can significantly impact our recall of past events, even if they are traumatic. Older adults, for instance, may be more sensitive to memory lapses and report them more frequently due to their heightened awareness of cognitive decline and the negative stereotype associated with aging. Memory can be influenced by psychological factors like anxiety and threat. On the other hand, memory athletes use techniques like the memory palace to hone their memory skills and remember large quantities of information. By understanding these factors and utilizing effective memory strategies, we can all improve our ability to remember things.

    • Using past experiences to improve memory through the technique of creating a memory palaceCreate mental associations between new information and familiar locations and items to enhance memory

      Our past experiences and familiar environments can be used to improve memory through the technique of creating a memory palace. This involves associating new information with specific locations and items in a familiar place. For instance, if you want to remember a list of items, you can create a mental path in a familiar location, like your house, and place each item at a specific stop along the way. This technique, which dates back to ancient Greek and Roman orators, uses the power of association to help recall unfamiliar information. Prior experiences can also contaminate memory, but they can also be used to enhance memory by creating connections between new and old information. So instead of letting past experiences hinder memory, we can use them to our advantage by creating vivid mental images and associations. For example, if I give you a list of five items - milk, eggs, olive oil, garlic, and pink cake frosting - you can create a mental path and place each item at a specific location along the way, such as a pink-frosted garlic cake in the kitchen. By using imagination and familiarity, we can make new information more memorable and easier to recall.

    • Islands of memories connected by bridgesUse unusual associations, space out learning, and ensure quality sleep to strengthen memory islands

      Our memories are not isolated entities but are interconnected through associations, making the context in which we learn and remember information crucial. This interconnectedness can be thought of as islands of memories, with bridges connecting related concepts. The use of unusual, interactive imagery and making associations can help us remember unrelated lists of items. However, these associations can also lead to errors. An effective way to improve memory retention is through practice tests and spacing out learning, rather than cramming before an exam. Additionally, memory consolidation occurs during sleep, emphasizing the importance of getting specific high-quality sleep to strengthen memories. Just as our bodies require rest and balance to engage in physical activity effectively, our brains also require a balance of interconnected processes for memory formation and retrieval. This physical change in the brain is crucial for making conscious experiences of recalled events. So, when trying to remember a grocery list or any other information, use unusual associations, space out your learning, and ensure you get enough quality sleep to strengthen your memory islands.

    • The Fallibility of Our MemoriesOur memories can be inaccurate and often don't align with reality, emphasizing the importance of understanding their complexities and the role of emotions like regret in shaping them.

      Our memories are not always an accurate reflection of reality. Ayanna Thomas, a psychologist, discussed the degradation of memories and ways to improve memory retention. During the conversation, the speaker mentioned milk, eggs, olive oil, garlic, and pink cake frosting, which they thought they would be asked to recall. This anecdote highlights the fallibility of our memories. Ayanna's research focuses on understanding why our recollections don't always align with reality. In the upcoming episode of Hidden Brain, the focus shifts to the emotion of regret. Regret is often seen as a negative emotion, but Ayanna emphasizes that it can be a positive force, helping us learn from our mistakes and grow. Yap Mura, a theoretical neuropsychologist, shared insights on memory research and the importance of replication studies. Overall, the discussion underscores the importance of understanding the complexities of our memories and emotions.

    Recent Episodes from Hidden Brain

    Making the World Sparkle Again

    Making the World Sparkle Again

    Across every domain of our lives, our minds have a tendency to get accustomed to things. In fact, the brain seems evolutionarily designed to focus on the new and unexpected, on novel threats and opportunities. In our daily lives, this means we take wonderful things for granted. We cease to appreciate amazing people, or the good fortune of being healthy. This week, neuroscientist Tali Sharot explains why we get used to things — and how to see with fresh eyes.

    Our podcast subscription, Hidden Brain+, is now available across platforms and devices. You can sign up for a free seven-day trial at support.hiddenbrain.org or apple.co/hiddenbrain. Your subscription provides key support to help us build you many more episodes of Hidden Brain. We’re truly grateful. 

    Hidden Brain
    enJune 24, 2024

    Our God-Shaped Brains

    Our God-Shaped Brains

    Some think of religious faith as just that: a leap of faith. But psychologists are increasingly filling in the gaps in our understanding of how beliefs shape — and are shaped by — the human mind. This week, psychologist Ara Norenzayan explores features in the brain that are tied to our capacity for faith. And he shows how all of us, both religious and non-religious people, can use this knowledge to find more meaning in our lives.

    For more of our reporting on religion and the mind, be sure to check out our episode "Creating God."
     

    Hidden Brain
    enJune 17, 2024

    Why You Feel Empty

    Why You Feel Empty

    Have you ever had an unexplainable feeling of emptiness? Life seems perfect - and yet - something is missing. This week, sociologist Corey Keyes helps us understand where feelings of emptiness come from, how to navigate them and why they're more common than we might assume.

    If you missed it, make sure to listen to last week's episode on Why Trying Too Hard Can Backfire On You. Thanks for listening!

    Hidden Brain
    enJune 10, 2024

    Why Trying Too Hard Can Backfire On You

    Why Trying Too Hard Can Backfire On You

    Thinking is a human superpower. On a daily basis, thinking and planning and effort bring us innumerable benefits. But like all aspects of human behavior, you can sometimes get too much of a good thing. This week, we talk with philosopher Ted Slingerland about techniques to prevent overthinking, and how we can cultivate the under-appreciated skill of letting go. 

    To hear more of our conversation with Ted Slingerland, be sure to check out our Hidden Brain+ episode with him, available now. You can join Hidden Brain+ via Patreon or Apple Podcasts. Thanks for listening! 

    Hidden Brain
    enJune 03, 2024

    Innovation 2.0: Do Less

    Innovation 2.0: Do Less

    The human drive to invent new things has led to pathbreaking achievements in medicine, science and society. But our desire to create can keep us from seeing one of the most powerful paths to progress: subtraction. In a favorite conversation from 2022, engineer Leidy Klotz shares how streamlining and simplifying is sometimes the best path to innovation. 

    Today's episode concludes our Innovation 2.0 series. If you've enjoyed these episodes, please tell a friend about them! They can find all of the stories in this series in this podcast feed, or at https://hiddenbrain.org/. Thanks for listening! 

    Hidden Brain
    enMay 27, 2024

    Innovation 2.0: Shortcuts and Speed Bumps

    Innovation 2.0: Shortcuts and Speed Bumps

    Most of us love to brainstorm with colleagues. But so often, our idea-generating sessions don't lead to anything tangible. Teams fill up walls with sticky notes about creative possibilities and suggestions for improvement, but nothing actually gets implemented. Some researchers even have a name for it: "innovation theater." This week, we explore the science of execution. Psychologist Bob Sutton tells us how to move from innovation theater . . . to actual innovation.

    You can find all the episodes in our Innovation 2.0 series in this podcast feed, or on our website, hiddenbrain.org

    Hidden Brain
    enMay 20, 2024

    Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have

    Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have

    Think about the last time you asked someone for something. Maybe you were nervous or worried about what the person would think of you. Chances are that you didn’t stop to think about the pressure you were exerting on that person. This week, we continue our Innovation 2.0 series with a 2020 episode about a phenomenon known as as “egocentric bias.” We talk with psychologist Vanessa Bohns about how this bias leads us astray, and how we can use this knowledge to ask for the things we need. 

    Did you catch the first two episodes in our Innovation 2.0 series? You can find them in this podcast feed or on our website. And if you're enjoying this series, please share it with a friend or family member. Thanks! 

    Hidden Brain
    enMay 13, 2024

    Innovation 2.0: Multiplying the Growth Mindset

    Innovation 2.0: Multiplying the Growth Mindset

    Have you ever been in a situation where you felt that people wrote you off? Maybe a teacher suggested you weren't talented enough to take a certain class, or a boss implied that you didn't have the smarts needed to handle a big project. In the latest in our "Innovation 2.0 series," we talk with Mary Murphy, who studies what she calls "cultures of genius." We'll look at how these cultures can keep people and organizations from thriving, and how we can create environments that better foster our growth.

    Do you know someone who'd find the ideas in today's episode to be useful? Please share it with them! And if you liked today's conversation, you might also like these classic Hidden Brain episodes: 

     The Edge Effect

    The Secret to Great Teams

    Dream Jobs

    Innovation 2.0: How Big Ideas Are Born

    Innovation 2.0: How Big Ideas Are Born

    Why is it so hard to guess where we're meant to be? To predict where we'll end up? Nearly all of us have had the experience of traveling down one road, only to realize it's not the road for us. At the University of Virginia, Saras Sarasvathy uses the lens of entrepreneurship to study how we plan and prepare for the future. We kick off our new "Innovation 2.0" series by talking with Saras about how we pursue goals and make decisions.

    Do you know someone who might benefit from our conversation with Saras about expert entrepreneurs? Please share it with them if so! And be sure to check out our other conversations about how to get out of ruts and figure out a path forward: 

    Who Do You Want to Be?

    You 2.0 : How to Break Out of a Rut

    Parents: Keep Out!

    Parents: Keep Out!

    If you're a parent or a teacher, you've probably wondered how to balance play and safety for the kids in your care. You don't want to put children in danger, but you also don't want to rob them of the joy of exploration. This week, we talk with psychologist Peter Gray about how this balance has changed — for parents and children alike — and what we can do about it.

    For more of our reporting on children and parents, check out these classic Hidden Brain episodes:

    Kinder-Gardening

    Bringing Up Baby 

     

    Related Episodes

    Ep. 213 - The Doctor Is In Series - Everything You Remember is False

    Ep. 213 - The Doctor Is In Series - Everything You Remember is False

    Welcome to the Social-Engineer Podcast: The Doctor Is In Series – where we will discuss understandings and developments in the field of psychology.

     

    In today’s episode, Chris and Abbie are discussing: False Memories. Although memory processes and systems usually operate reliably, they are sometimes prone to distortions and illusions. Today’s discussion will examine how and why this happens. [June 5, 2023]

     

    00:00 - Intro

    00:20 - Dr. Abbie Maroño Intro

    01:02 - Intro Links

    -          Social-Engineer.com - http://www.social-engineer.com/

    -          Managed Voice Phishing - https://www.social-engineer.com/services/vishing-service/

    -          Managed Email Phishing - https://www.social-engineer.com/services/se-phishing-service/

    -          Adversarial Simulations - https://www.social-engineer.com/services/social-engineering-penetration-test/

    -          Social-Engineer channel on SLACK - https://social-engineering-hq.slack.com/ssb

    -          CLUTCH - http://www.pro-rock.com/

    -          innocentlivesfoundation.org - http://www.innocentlivesfoundation.org/

    07:43 - The Topic of the Day: False Memories

    08:11 - Defining Our Memories

    10:17 - Challenging Your Reality

    11:48 - Remember the Good Times

    13:01 - The Exception

    15:07 - Unintentional Ego Inflation

    17:27 - Putting it in Context

    18:46 - The Dangers of Distorting Memories

    23:19 - Not-So-Total Recall

    25:40 - Repression vs Suppression

    28:35 - Eyewitness Error

    32:10 - Shameless Plug: Ep. 134

    -          Altered Memories and Alternate Realities with Dr. Elizabeth Loftus

    34:14 - Emotional Influence

    37:22 - How Accurate Are You???

    39:56 - Emotional Defense

    44:35 - Belief System

    47:48 - Don't Be Certain (Because You're Not)

    49:31 - Confirmation Bias

    52:39 - Simple Does Not Equal Easy

    54:08 - Shades of Grey

    56:38 - Wrap Up

    56:58 - Next Month: Deception Detection

    57:45 - Outro

    -          www.social-engineer.com

    -          www.innocentlivesfoundation.org

     

    Find us online:

    -          Twitter: https://twitter.com/abbiejmarono

    -          LinkedIn: linkedin.com/in/dr-abbie-maroño-phd-35ab2611a

    -          Twitter: https://twitter.com/humanhacker

    -          LinkedIn: linkedin.com/in/christopherhadnagy

     

    References:

    Damiano, C., & Walther, D. B. (2019). Distinct roles of eye movements during memory encoding and retrieval. Cognition, 184, 119-129.

    Robins, S. K. (2019). Confabulation and constructive memory. Synthese, 196, 2135-2151.

    Schacter, D. L. (2022). Constructive memory: past and future. Dialogues in clinical neuroscience.

    Murphy, G., Loftus, E. F., Grady, R. H., Levine, L. J., & Greene, C. M. (2019). False memories for fake news during Ireland’s abortion referendum. Psychological science, 30(10), 1449-1459.

    Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (2020). In human memory, good can be stronger than bad. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(1), 86-91.

    Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., & Patihis, L. (2022). What science tells us about false and repressed memories. Memory, 30(1), 16-21.

    Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American psychologist, 48(5), 518.

    Anderson, M. C., & Hulbert, J. C. (2021). Active forgetting: Adaptation of memory by prefrontal control. Annual review of psychology, 72, 1-36.

    Loftus, E. F., & Pickrell, J. E. (1995). The formation of false memories. Psychiatric annals, 25(12), 720-725.

    Otgaar, H., Candel, I., Merckelbach, H., & Wade, K. A. (2009). Abducted by a UFO: Prevalence information affects young children's false memories for an implausible event. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 23(1), 115-125.

    Otgaar, H., Candel, I., Scoboria, A., & Merckelbach, H. (2010). Script knowledge enhances the development of children’s false memories. Acta Psychologica, 133(1), 57-63.

    Improve your sense of direction

    Improve your sense of direction
    Do you feel like you always need to use GPS to get around? Never quite sure if it's a left or a right turn — even though you just looked at a map? It can be anxiety-inducing to get around when you don't have a great sense of direction. This episode, experts explain how to gain more confidence while navigating.

    Learn more about sponsor message choices: podcastchoices.com/adchoices

    NPR Privacy Policy

    Ghostkeeping

    Ghostkeeping
    In this episode, Jim & Tanya explore a concept that came to Jim in a dream, the night before recording this episode. This is a raw discussion about the idea of "Ghostkeeping," - when your reality doesn't match up with your perception of the way things should be, or when you are actively holding onto a memory that is holding you back. Jim & Tanya do their best to "flesh it out," (pun intended) in this show.

    Give it a listen! Let us know if you have an idea about what it means. Join our facebook group: https://www.facebook.com/groups/ThePowerToBeYou to join in the discussion, or find us on social media:

    IG: @jim_varley   

    IG: @trvarley

    Twitter: @jhvarley

    Twitter:   @TVarley921